Vocalizaciones asociadas al comportamiento colonial de Cacicus chrysonotus leucoramphus (Icteridae) en Colombia
HTML
PDF
XML

Palabras clave

Birds
Bioacoustics
Coloniality
Social behavior
Song Avifauna
Bioacústica
Canto
Colonialidad
Comportamiento social

Cómo citar

Guaitarilla, D. A., Ortiz-Pérez, J. P., Calderón-Leytón, J. J., Gómez-Martínez, M. A., Trujillo-Torres, C. M., & Fernández, R. A. (2021). Vocalizaciones asociadas al comportamiento colonial de Cacicus chrysonotus leucoramphus (Icteridae) en Colombia. Biota Colombiana, 22(1), 133–148. https://doi.org/10.21068/c2021.v22n01a09

Resumen

Con el objetivo de evaluar la asociación entre las vocalizaciones y los comportamientos del cacique montano norteño (Cacicus chrysonotus leucoramphus), se tomaron registros acústicos y conductuales de la especie en cuatro localidades de los Andes de Nariño, en el sur de Colombia. Se registraron seis tipos de vocalizaciones asociadas a por lo menos una de las tres categorías conductuales identificadas (vigilancia, alerta y defensa territorial), y una vocalización registrada en un único evento de cortejo. Vocalizaciones con valores más altos de ancho de banda y modulación de frecuencia resultaron asociadas a defensa territorial, mientras que vocalizaciones con valores bajos de ancho de banda y poca modulación cumplieron funciones de vigilancia y alerta. Los resultados indican similitudes en el comportamiento vocal y social con respecto a estudios en otras especies como Cacicus cela Cacicus haemorrhousC. c. leucoramphus utiliza vocalizaciones específicas en un determinado contexto conductual, pudiendo algunas de ellas ser reutilizadas en varios comportamientos. Este estudio describe por primera vez el contexto social en el que se asocian las vocalizaciones y conductas en esta especie.

https://doi.org/10.21068/c2021.v22n01a09
HTML
PDF
XML

Referencias

Alcock, J. (2001). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach. Sunderland, U. S.: Sinauer Associates.

Alexander, R. D. (1974). The Evolution of social behavior. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 5, 325-383.

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.05.110174.001545.

Bioacoustics Research Program. (2011). Raven Pro: interactive sound analysis software. Version 1.5. [Ithaca (NY)]: The Cornell Lab of Ornithology.

Beauchamp, G. (2015). Animal Vigilance: Monitoring Predators and Competitors. New York, U.S.: Academic Press.

Boucherie, P. H., Loretto, M., Massen, J. J. M., & Bugnyar, T. (2019). What constitutes “social complexity” and “social intelligence” in birds? Lessons from ravens. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73, (12), 2-14.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2607-2

Bradbury, J.W. & Vehrencamp, S. L. (2011). Principles of Animal Communication. Sunderland, U. S.: Sinauer Associates.

Budney, G. & Grotke, R. (2013). Techniques for audio recording vocalizations of tropical birds. New York, U. S.: Library of Natural Sounds, Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology.

Carazo, P. & Font, E. (2010). Putting information back into biological communication. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 23, 661-669.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.01944.x

Catchpole, C. K. (1982). The evolution of bird sounds in relation to mating and spacing behavior. En: Kroodsma D. K. & Miller, E. H., (eds.). Acoustic Communication in Birds. New York, U. S.: Academic Press.

Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. (2008). Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. New York, U. S.: Cambridge University Press.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, U. S.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Duijns, S. & Piersma, T. (2014). Interference competition in a sexually dimorphic shorebird: prey behaviour explains intraspecific competition. Animal Behaviour, 92, 195-201.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.04.007

Feekes F. (1982). Song mimesis within colonies of Cacicus c. cela (Icteridae, Aves). A colonial password?. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 58, 119-152.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1982.tb00312.x.

Fraga, R. M. (2008). Phylogeny and behavioral evolution in the family Icteridae. Ornitología Neotropical, 19, 61-71.

Fraga, R. (2020). Mountain Cacique (Cacicus chrysonotus), version 1.0. In Birds of the World (S. M. Billerman, B. K. Keeney, P. G. Rodewald, and T. S. Schulenberg, Editors). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA.

https://doi.org/10.2173/bow.moucac1.01

Freeberg, T.M., Dunbar, R.I., & Ord, T.J., (2012). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Proceedings of the Royal. Society B: Biological Sciences 367, 1785-1801.

https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0213

Gil, D. & Gahr, M. (2002). The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 17, 133-141.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02410-2.

Greig, E. I., Price, J. J., & Pruett-Jones, S. (2013). Song evolution in Maluridae: influences of natural and sexual selection on acoustic structure. Emu-Austral Ornithology, 113(3), 270-281.

https://doi.org/10.1071/MU12078

Gross, M. R. & MacMillan, A. M. (1981). Predation and the evolution of colonial nesting in bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 8,163-174.

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299826

Guilford, T. & Dawkins, M. S. (1991). Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Animal Behaviour, 42, 1-14.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80600-1

Jaramillo, A. & Burke, P. (1999). New World Blackbirds: The Icterids. Princeton, U. S.: Princeton University Press.

Jungwirth, A., Josi, D., Walker, J., & Taborsky, M. (2015). Benefits of coloniality: communal defence saves anti-predator effort in cooperative breeders. Functional Ecology, 29(9), 1218-1224.

https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12430

Koening, W. D., Mumme, R. L., Stanback, M. T. & Pitelka, F. A. (1995) Patterns and consequences of egg destruction among joint-nesting acorn woodpeckers. Animal Behaviour, 50, 607-621.

http://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(95)80123-5.

Ladich, F., & Winkler, H. (2017). Acoustic communication in terrestrial and aquatic vertebrates. Journal of Experimental Biology, 220(13), 2306-2317.

https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.132944

Leak, J., & Robinson, S. K. (1989). Notes on the social behavior and mating system of the Casqued Oropendola. The Wilson Bulletin, 101, 134-137.

Leighton, G. M. (2017). Cooperative breeding influences the number and type of vocalizations in avian lineages. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1868), 20171508.

https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1508

López-Martínez, M. L. & Madroñero-Palacios, S. M. (2015). Estado trófico de un lago tropical de alta montaña: caso laguna de La Cocha. Ciencia e Ingeniería Neogranadina, 25, 21-42.

http://doi.org/10.18359/rcin.1430

Marler, P. (1967) Animal communication signals: We are beginning to understand how the structure of animal signals relates to the function they serve. Science, 157, 769-774.

https://doi.org/10.1126/science.157.3790.769

Martin, P. & Bateson, P. (2007). Measuring Behaviour: An introductory Guide. Cambridge, U. K.: Cambridge University Press.

Miles, M. C., & Fuxjager, M. J. (2018). Animal choreography of song and dance: a case study in the Montezuma Oropendola, Psarocolius montezuma. Animal behaviour, 140, 99-107.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.04.006

Partan, S. R., & Marler, P. (2005). Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. The American Naturalist, 166(2), 231-245.

https://doi.org/10.1086/431246

Payne, R. B. (1979). Song structure, behaviour, and sequence of song types in a population of village indigobirds, Vidua chalybeata. Animal Behaviour, 27, 997-1013.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(79)90047-2

Podos, J. (1997). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution, 51, 537-551.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb02441.x

Price, J. J. & Lanyon, S. M. (2004). Patterns of song evolution and sexual selection in the oropendolas and caciques. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 485-497.

https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arh040

Price, J. J., Johnson, K. P. & Clayton, D. H. (2004). The evolution of echolocation in swiftlets. Journal of Avian Biology, 35, 135-143.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0908-8857.2004.03182.x

Price, J. J., Earnshaw, S. M. & Webster, M. S. (2006). Montezuma oropendolas modify a component of song constrained by body size during vocal contests. Animal Behaviour, 71, 799-807.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.025

Price, J. J., Friedman, N. R. & Omland, K. E. (2007). Song and plumage evolution in the New World orioles (Icterus) show similar lability and convergence in patterns. Evolution, 61, 850-863.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00082.x

Price, J. J. & Whalen, L. M. (2009). Plumage evolution in the oropendolas and caciques: different divergence rates in polygynous and monogamous taxa. Evolution, 63, 2985-2998.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00765.x

Ríos-Chelén, A. A., McDonald, A. N., Berger, A., Perry, A. C., Krakauer, A. H. & Patricelli, G. L. (2017). Do birds vocalize at higher pitch in noise, or is it a matter of measurement?. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 71, 29.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-016-2243-7

Robinson, S. K. (1985a). Fighting and assessment in the Yellow-rumped Cacique (Cacicus cela). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 8, 39-44.

http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299236

Robinson, S. K. (1985b). Coloniality in the yellow-rumped Cacique (Cacicus cela) as a defense against nest predators. The Auk, 102, 506-519.

https://doi.org/10.1093/auk/102.3.506

Robinson, S. K. (1986). Benefits, costs, and determinants of dominance in a polygynous oriole. Animal Behaviour, 34, 241-255.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(86)90029-1

Robinson, S. K. (1997). Birds of a Peruvian oxbow lake: populations, resources, predation, and social behavior. Ornithological Monographs, 48, 613-639.

https://doi.org/10.2307/40157558

Rolland, C., Danchin, E. & de Fraipont, M. (1998). The evolution of coloniality in birds in relation to food, habitat, predation, and life-history traits: a comparative analysis. The American naturalist, 151, 514-29.

https://doi.org/10.1086/286137

Rubenstein, D. R & Alcock, J. (2018). Animal behavior. Sunderland, M.A. Sinauer Associates.

Santos, E. S. & Macedo, R. H. (2011). Load lightening in southern lapwings: group-living mothers lay smaller eggs than pair-living mothers. Ethology, 117(6), 547-555.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01905.x

Schradin, C., König, B., & Pillay, N. (2010). Reproductive competition favours solitary living while ecological constraints impose group-living in African striped mice. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79(3), 515-521.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01651.x

Searcy W. A. & Nowicky, S. (2005). The Evolution of Animal Communication Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems. Princeton, U. S.: Princeton University Press.

Statgraphics Centurion, X. V. I. (2009). Statpoint technologies. INC. version, 16, 17.

Stutchbury, B. J. & Morton, E. S. (2001). Behavioral Ecology of Tropical Birds. San Diego, U. S.: Academic Press.

Thieltges, H., Henry, L., Biquand, V., Du Bosq, S., Hervé, M., Hausberger, M. & Deleporte, P. (2013). Dialects in short songs of two species of colonial caciques (Cacicus). En: Thieltges, H. (2013). Distribution spatiale, stabilité et perception des dialectes chez deux espèces d’oiseaux guyanais (Cacicus cela et Cacicus haemorrhous) (Tesis doctoral). Université Rennes, Rennes, Francia.

Thieltges, H., Henry, L. Biquand, V. & Deleporte, P. (2014). Short-term variations of dialects in short songs of two species of colonial caciques (Cacicus) Acta Acustica United with Acustica, 100, 759-766.

http://doi.org/10.3813/AAA.918755

Tirado, E. A., Collado- Corona, M. A., & Morales- Martínez, J. J. (2004). Comunicación y comportamiento auditivo obtenidos por medio de los potenciales evocados auditivos en mamíferos, aves, anfibios y reptiles. Cirugía y Cirujanos, 72, 309-315. Recuperado de: https://www.medigraphic.com

Trainer, J. M. (1987). Behavioral associations of song types during aggressive interactions among male yellow-rumped Caciques. The Condor, 89, 731-738.

https://doi.org/10.2307/1368519

Trainer, J. M. (1988). Singing organization during aggressive interactions among male Yellow-Rumped Caciques. The Condor, 90, 681.

https://doi.org/10.2307/1368358

Trainer, J. M. (1989). Cultural evolution in song dialects of Yellow-rumped Caciques in Panama. Ethology, 80, 190-204.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1989.tb00739.x

Vallet, E., Beme, I. & Kreutzer, M. (1998). Two-note syllables in canary songs elicit high levels of sexual display. Animal Behaviour, 55, 291-297.

https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0631

Webster, M. S., Ligon, R. A., & Leighton, G. M. (2018). Social costs are an underappreciated force for honest signalling in animal aggregations. Animal Behaviour, 143, 167-176.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.12.006

Wiley, R. H. (1976). Communication and spatial relationships in a colony of common grackles. Animal Behaviour, 24, 570-584.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80070-X

Wittwer, B., Hefetz, A., Simon, T., Murphy, L. E., Elgar, M. A., Pierce, N. E., & Kocher, S. D. (2017). Solitary bees reduce investment in communication compared with their social relatives. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(25), 6569-6574.

https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620780114

Creative Commons License

Esta obra está bajo una licencia internacional Creative Commons Atribución-NoComercial-SinDerivadas 4.0.

Derechos de autor 2021 Instituto de Investigación de Recursos Biológicos Alexander Von Humboldt

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.